Models of Modals (eBook)

From Pragmatics and Corpus Linguistics to Machine Learning
eBook Download: EPUB
2023
280 Seiten
De Gruyter (Verlag)
978-3-11-073425-6 (ISBN)

Lese- und Medienproben

Models of Modals - Ilse Depraetere, Bert Cappelle, Martin Hilpert, Ludovic De Cuypere, Mathieu Dehouck, Pascal Denis, Susanne Flach, Natalia Grabar, Cyril Grandin, Thierry Hamon, Clemens Hufeld, Benoît Leclercq, Hans-Jörg Schmid
Systemvoraussetzungen
139,95 inkl. MwSt
  • Download sofort lieferbar
  • Zahlungsarten anzeigen

Modal verbs in English communicate delicate shades of meaning, there being a large range of verbs both on the necessity side (must, have to, should, ought to, need, need to) and the possibility side (can, may, could, might, be able to). They therefore constitute excellent test ground to apply and compare different methodologies that can lay bare the factors that drive the speaker's choice of modal verb. This book is not merely concerned with a purely grammatical description of the use of modal verbs, but aims at advancing our understanding of lexical and grammatical units in general and of linguistic methodologies to explore these. It thus involves a genuine effort to compare, assess and combine a variety of approaches. It complements the leading descriptive qualitative work on modal verbs by testing a diverse range of quantitative methods, while not ignoring qualitative issues pertaining to the semantics-pragmatics interface. Starting from a critical assessment of what constitutes the meaning of modal verbs, different types of empirical studies (usage-based, data-driven and experimental), drawing considerably on the same data sets, shows how method triangulation can contribute to an enhanced understanding. Due attention is also given to individual variation as well as the degree to which modals can predict L2 proficiency level.



Ilse Depraetere (Université de Lille), Bert Cappelle (Université de Lille), Martin Hilpert (Université de Neuch?tel),

Ludovic De Cuypere (UGent, VUB), Mathieu Dehouck (CNRS, Lattice), Pascal Denis (Inria, MAGNET), Susanne Flach (Zürich), Natalia Grabar (CNRS, STL), Cyril Grandin, Thierry Hamon (Paris 13, LISN), Clemens Hufeld (LMU München), Benoît Leclercq (Paris 8), Hans-Jörg Schmid (LMU München)

Introduction


Ilse Depraetere
Bert Cappelle
Martin Hilpert

1 Modal verbs and why they have attracted a lot of attention


This volume applies different approaches to an area of English linguistics that is particularly challenging, that of the meaning of English modals. Our aim is to arrive at a better and richer understanding by combining insights from methods and theories among which there has as yet not been much interaction. In the course of doing so, we test their relative strengths and potential shortcomings, and show in what ways they complement each other and can shed new light on the topic at stake.

Modality, the linguistic expression of possibility and necessity, is a topic that has inspired a lot of research. This is hardly surprising for a lexical-grammatical area that pertains to an indispensable aspect of human cognition. As Narrog (2012: 1) puts it, “[h]uman beings constantly imagine things that are not real but still possible or even contrary to facts. They continually evaluate the reality status of states-of-affairs, or urge or allow interlocutors to bring about states-of-affairs in reality.” Take any moderate-sized stretch of spoken or written text and you will find that as people, we cannot help but communicate our natural interest in what is possible and what is necessary. A common way of expressing this pervasive concern is by means of modal verbs. To illustrate this, we provide in (1a-d) just some of the sentences with modal verbs that appeared on the website of a conference on English linguistics:1

(1)
  1. ISLE6 preliminary program summary 31 August 2020 – subject to change Wednesday, 3 June 2020 9/10–12 Workshops, Part 1 (individual workshops may start later)

  2. The session chair will introduce the speaker(s) and you may begin your presentation.

  3. In total, around five minutes of kneading should be fine. [from a page devoted to the host country of the conference, with recipes of some local food]

  4. The length of your abstract should not exceed 500 words (excluding references).

In (1a-b), the modal verb may expresses shades of possibility; in (1c-d), the modal verb should expresses kinds of necessity. ‘Modality’ is an important, possibly universal, functional concept; it has been defined not only in terms of forms that represent situations as possible or necessary, but also, in more general terms, as forms that represent a situation as less than factual. Following Van der Auwera and Plungian (1998), we subscribe to the former, more restrictive approach. This implies that volition, which does not enter, in a straightforward way, into the possibility-necessity paradigm, will not be in the foreground in the chapters of this book. In any case, given the ubiquity of modal verbs in discourse, there is little wonder that they have been of prominent interest in linguistic research. Two more specific reasons can explain why linguists have been fascinated by them.

A first main reason for linguists’ attention to modal verbs is their semantic versatility. In (1a) and (1c), the modal verbs may and should both express epistemic modality, reflecting on the speaker’s perceived likelihood of the situation being the case (moderately likely in the case of may and very likely in the case of should), based on some knowledge of reality the speaker has.2 In (1b) and (1d), the same two modals express root, or non-epistemic, modality: the utterer is not concerned here with any degree of certainty as to the truth of the non-modalized proposition, but more directly with factors that impact on the actualization of the situation expressed. The existence of permission makes the situation of beginning a presentation possible in (1b); the existence of a formal requirement makes it necessary for participants’ abstracts not to exceed a word limit in (1d). Again, possibility and necessity are involved, but it should be clear that root interpretations of these two modals differ substantially from the same two modals’ epistemic readings. How to deal insightfully with the availability of these two radically different semantic notions of modal verbs has, understandably, been a major analytical challenge. There is some controversy, for example, as to how distinct these notions are. In the same recipe from which (1c) was drawn, we find this example:

(2)

Use your hands to knead the dough in the bowl. It should become elastic.

Is this use of should epistemic (the writer considering it quite likely that the dough will become elastic when kneading it), non-epistemic (the writer pointing out that it is important for the dough to become elastic), or some kind of mixture? While the exact interpretation may not matter a great deal in this specific context, the question is whether such an example suggests that epistemic and root (or non-epistemic) meanings can coexist. Coates (1983: 17) has discussed such cases under the heading of ‘merger’. The context here simply does not allow us to decide with absolute certainty which of the two interpretations, which really are conceptually different, was intended by the writer. (In this case, it was probably the non-epistemic reading, as recipes are essentially sets of instructions.) We shall have more to say about the notion of merger in Chapter 1.

A second important reason for the popularity of modal verbs in linguistic research is that for any single intended modal meaning, broadly defined (e.g., epistemic possibility; non-epistemic necessity), there are typically several modal verbs that can be used, with often little discernible semantic difference. Thus, in the examples given above in (1a-d), we could substitute other modal verbs and thereby hardly change the meaning of the sentences:

(3)
  1. ISLE6 preliminary program summary 31 August 2020 – subject to change Wednesday, 3 June 2020

    9/10–12 Workshops, Part 1 (individual workshops may / might / could start later)

  2. The session chair will introduce the speaker(s) and you may / can begin your presentation.

  3. In total, around five minutes of kneading should / will / ought to be fine.

  4. The length of your abstract should not / must not / ought not to / may not / cannot exceed 500 words (excluding references).

The availability of multiple expressions for a single meaning raises familiar linguistic questions relating to variation and competition. In this respect, note that while the focus in this book is on modal verbs (sometimes just called ‘modals’) in the English language, there are various other formal realizations of modality. Consider the following sentence, in which the main clause, which does not have a finite verb, expresses non-epistemic necessity thanks to best (to):

(4)

If you think a concept like white privilege has some validity, best to explain carefully what you mean by it, and what you don’t. (The Economist, 1 July 2021; boldface added – ID, BC, MH)

For an overview of the kinds of markers that have been addressed under the heading of modality, see, for example, Portner (2009: 2–8). Modal adverbs and adjectives, specifically, have been studied by Hoye (1997), Simon-Vandenbergen and Aijmer (2007) and Van Linden (2012).

Modality, and modal verbs in particular, thus constitute the classic double problem of (i) single forms having multiple meanings and (ii) single meanings being expressible with multiple forms. This, and their centrality in thought and language, make them an ideal testing ground for fundamental semasiological and onomasiological research into variation in meaning and form. Accordingly, modality has been studied within various theoretical models, both linguistic and philosophical in nature, and from various research perspectives. In linguistics, modality has been dealt with from a formal semantic approach (e.g. Kratzer 2012, Portner 2009, 2018), within Relevance Theory (e.g. Papafragou 2000), and within cognitive linguistics and Construction Grammar (e.g. Talmy 1988, Sweetser 1990, Langacker 2003, Mortelmans 2007, Boogaart 2009, Boogaart and Fortuin 2016). Different research practices and concerns can be distinguished as well. For instance, the diachrony of...

Erscheint lt. Verlag 27.4.2023
Reihe/Serie ISSN
ISSN
Topics in English Linguistics [TiEL]
Topics in English Linguistics [TiEL]
Zusatzinfo 22 b/w and 17 col. ill., 42 b/w tbl.
Sprache englisch
Themenwelt Geisteswissenschaften Sprach- / Literaturwissenschaft Sprachwissenschaft
Schlagworte Corpus Linguistics • Corpuslinguistik • English Modals • English Modals, Pragmatics, Corpus Linguistics, Machine Learning • machine learning • Modalverben • Pragmatics • Pragmatik
ISBN-10 3-11-073425-7 / 3110734257
ISBN-13 978-3-11-073425-6 / 9783110734256
Haben Sie eine Frage zum Produkt?
EPUBEPUB (Wasserzeichen)
Größe: 3,9 MB

DRM: Digitales Wasserzeichen
Dieses eBook enthält ein digitales Wasser­zeichen und ist damit für Sie persona­lisiert. Bei einer missbräuch­lichen Weiter­gabe des eBooks an Dritte ist eine Rück­ver­folgung an die Quelle möglich.

Dateiformat: EPUB (Electronic Publication)
EPUB ist ein offener Standard für eBooks und eignet sich besonders zur Darstellung von Belle­tristik und Sach­büchern. Der Fließ­text wird dynamisch an die Display- und Schrift­größe ange­passt. Auch für mobile Lese­geräte ist EPUB daher gut geeignet.

Systemvoraussetzungen:
PC/Mac: Mit einem PC oder Mac können Sie dieses eBook lesen. Sie benötigen dafür die kostenlose Software Adobe Digital Editions.
eReader: Dieses eBook kann mit (fast) allen eBook-Readern gelesen werden. Mit dem amazon-Kindle ist es aber nicht kompatibel.
Smartphone/Tablet: Egal ob Apple oder Android, dieses eBook können Sie lesen. Sie benötigen dafür eine kostenlose App.
Geräteliste und zusätzliche Hinweise

Buying eBooks from abroad
For tax law reasons we can sell eBooks just within Germany and Switzerland. Regrettably we cannot fulfill eBook-orders from other countries.

Mehr entdecken
aus dem Bereich
Digitale Produkte und Services mit Zeichentheorie besser verstehen …

von Klaus M. Bernsau

eBook Download (2024)
Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden (Verlag)
29,99