Changes in Sensory Motor Behavior in Aging -

Changes in Sensory Motor Behavior in Aging (eBook)

eBook Download: PDF
1995 | 1. Auflage
390 Seiten
Elsevier Science (Verlag)
978-0-08-052884-7 (ISBN)
Systemvoraussetzungen
173,37 inkl. MwSt
  • Download sofort lieferbar
  • Zahlungsarten anzeigen
Recently, studies on aging processes and age-related changes in behavior have been expanding considerably, probably due to the dramatic changes observed in the demographics. This increase in the overall age and proportion of elderly people has heightened the severity of problems associated with the safety and well-being of elderly persons in everyday life. Many researchers working on motor control have thus focused more intensely on the effects of age on motor control. This new avenue of research has led to programs for alleviating or delaying the specific sensory-motor limitations encountered by the elderly (e.g. falls) in an attempt to make the elderly more autonomous.

The aggregation of studies from different perspectives is often fascinating, especially when the same field can serve as a common ground between researchers. Nearly all contributors to this book work on sensory-motor aging, they represent a large range of affiliations and backgrounds including psychology, neurobiology, cognitive sciences, kinesiology, neuropsychology, neuropharmacology, motor performance, physical therapy, exercise science, and human development. Addressing age-related behavioral changes can also furnish some crucial reflections in the debate about motor coordination: aging is the product of both maturational and environmental processes, and studies on aging must determine how the intricate interrelationships between these processes evolve. The study of aging makes it possible to determine how compensatory mechanisms, operating on different subsystems and each aging at its own rate, compensate for biological degenerations and changing external demands. This volume will contribute to demonstrating that the study of the aging process raises important theoretical questions.


Recently, studies on aging processes and age-related changes in behavior have been expanding considerably, probably due to the dramatic changes observed in the demographics. This increase in the overall age and proportion of elderly people has heightened the severity of problems associated with the safety and well-being of elderly persons in everyday life. Many researchers working on motor control have thus focused more intensely on the effects of age on motor control. This new avenue of research has led to programs for alleviating or delaying the specific sensory-motor limitations encountered by the elderly (e.g. falls) in an attempt to make the elderly more autonomous.The aggregation of studies from different perspectives is often fascinating, especially when the same field can serve as a common ground between researchers. Nearly all contributors to this book work on sensory-motor aging; they represent a large range of affiliations and backgrounds including psychology, neurobiology, cognitive sciences, kinesiology, neuropsychology, neuropharmacology, motor performance, physical therapy, exercise science, and human development. Addressing age-related behavioral changes can also furnish some crucial reflections in the debate about motor coordination: aging is the product of both maturational and environmental processes, and studies on aging must determine how the intricate interrelationships between these processes evolve. The study of aging makes it possible to determine how compensatory mechanisms, operating on different subsystems and each aging at its own rate, compensate for biological degenerations and changing external demands. This volume will contribute to demonstrating that the study of the aging process raises important theoretical questions.

Cover 1
Contents 14
Preface 6
Acknowledgements 10
Contributors 12
Chapter 1. Age-related slowing in movement parameterization studies: Not what you might think 16
Chapter 2. Control of simple arm movements in the elderly 42
Chapter 3. Slowness, variability, and modulations of gait in healthy elderly 68
Chapter 4. Aging and coordination from the dynamic pattern perspective 104
Chapter 5. Posture control and muscle proprioception in the elderly 148
Chapter 6. Posture and gait in healthy elderly individuals and survivors of stroke 178
Chapter 7. Tests in rodents for assessing sensorimotor performance during aging 216
Chapter 8. Attentional demands for walking: Age-related changes 250
Chapter 9. Visual control of obstacle avoidance during locomotion: Strategies in young children, young and older adults 272
Chapter 10. Constraints on prehension: A framework for studying the effects of aging 294
Chapter 11. Age, perceived health, and specific and nonspecific measures of processing speed 330
Chapter 12. Balance control in older adults: Training effects on balance control and the integration of balance control into walking 354
Author Index 384
Subject Index 398

Age-Related Slowing in Movement Parameterization Studies: Not what you Might Think


Paul C. Amrhein*    University of New Mexico
* Correspondence should be sent to Paul C. Amrhein, Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Logan Hall, Terrace and Redondo Streets, NE, Albuquerque, NM 87131, U.S.A. email address: amrhein@hydra.unm.edu

Abstract


In this chapter, the nature of age-related slowing in speeded motor performance is explored. In particular, experiments assessing movement parameterization are reviewed. In these studies, specific movement parameters (e.g., arm, direction, extent) comprising a motor program are assessed concerning their preparation, maintenance, restructuring and execution within a movement plan. An advantage of movement parameterization studies is that they assess cognitive processing latency to assess a movement response (reaction time, RT) distinct from the latency to complete the movement response (movement time, MT). In general, most speeded tasks assess both of these latencies in aggregate (and refer to this aggregate latency as simply “RT”). As such, parameterization studies allow a test of prevailing response slowing theories of aging using components of task performance. Separate “Brinley plot” regressions of RT and Total Time (TT, TT = RT + MT) from these studies reveals additive slowing, but nominal (if any) multiplicative slowing. Moreover, the intercept difference between the best-fitting RT and TT lines validates the additive impact of MT in these studies. Even at a global level, these studies are inconsistent with claims of negligible additive slowing (i.e., small positive or negative intercept), but substantial multiplicative slowing (slope approximating 2.0) for “nonlexical” tasks espoused by General Slowing theorists (e.g., Lima, Myerson, & Hale, 1991). In addition, review of the individual studies indicates what the Brinley plot approach misses: Age Group × Condition interactions from some of these studies actually indicate speed increases in elderly relative to young subjects, due to apparent differences in parameter preparation maintenance and restructuring processes for the two age groups.

Key words

Aging

aimed movement

Brinley plot

movement time

reaction time

slowing

INTRODUCTION


One of the staple, if not classic, methodologies used to study the effects of aging on human performance has been the reaction time (RT) task (see, e.g., Salthouse, 1985; Welford, 1959, 1977; Spirduso & MacRae, 1990). In particular, two reaction time tasks have been used extensively: Simple reaction time (SRT) and choice reaction time (CRT). Based, respectively, on Donders' (1869/1969) Type A and B tasks, they provide a means to separately assess age effects on sensory-motor and intervening cognitive processes (Dawson, 1988; Teichner & Krebs, 1974). As such, they provide a useful way to assess at a process level the pervasive response slowing seen in older persons (see Botwinick, 1984; Goggin & Stelmach, 1990; Welford, 1977).

In the SRT task, a pre-specified stimulus is presented and the subject responds with a pre-instructed response. (A variant of this task is where the stimulus is presented but subjects respond upon a latent “GO” signal; for example, see the delayed pronunciation task of Balota & Duchek, 1988.) By knowing the stimulus and the response to it, subjects are likely to prepare this response prior to actually receiving the stimulus (or “GO” signal). In a typical CRT task, subjects respond to one of a number of stimuli with a pre-instructed response unique to each potential stimulus. SRT and CRT tasks share perceptual and motor aspects in their task demands; that is, in both tasks (excluding the latent “GO” signal version), subjects must detect that a stimulus has been presented, and (including the latent “GO” signal version) the corresponding response must be prepared and executed. What distinguishes SRT and CRT tasks is the uncertainty concerning which stimulus is actually presented. Whereas there is no stimulus uncertainty for the SRT task, there is for the CRT task. As numerous studies have reported over the years, increases in this uncertainty yield corresponding increases in response latency across the adult lifespan (see, e.g., Kausler, 1991; Salthouse, 1985; Welford, 1959, 1977).

Many motor performance tasks are built upon SRT and CRT task methodologies. Indeed, SRT and CRT tasks typically require a manual (i.e., aimed movement) response. In the SRT task, response parameters (concerning which finger, hand, arm, foot or leg will be used) are prepared by the subject prior to target stimulus onset (see e.g., Amrhein, Stelmach, & Goggin, 1991). In the CRT task, by contrast, such preparation does not appear to occur (Amrhein et al., 1991; Klapp, Wyatt, & Lingo, 1974). Thus, SRT and CRT tasks actually represent two extremes on the scale of response preparation, and as such represent useful reference points when studying movement plan preparation, maintenance, restructuring and execution. While most SRT and CRT studies have assessed response initiation (reaction time, RT) and execution (movement time, MT) in aggregate (but still refer to the data as “RT” even though it might be better referred to as “Total Time”, TT), there have been some studies which have used RT/MT assessment.

Methodologically, what distinguishes the larger set of “RT” from the smaller set of “RT/MT” studies is that subjects in the former set simply press a target button upon stimulus response, often with little experimental control over the initial resting location of their responding body part, whereas in the latter set, upon stimulus presentation, subjects release a button (often called a “Home button” or “Home key”), and then move to press a target button. In the aging literature, these RT/MT studies include: Amrhein et al. (1991), Amrhein, Von Dras, and Anderson (1993), Clarkson (1978), Goggin, Stelmach, and Amrhein (1989), Larish and Stelmach (1982), Spirduso (1975), and Goggin (1988), and Amrhein (1988), and Garcia-Colera (1987), Singleton (1954), Szafran (1951), and Welford (1959, 1977). Generally, these studies have revealed slower RTs and MTs for older (e.g., age range 50-87 years) relative to younger (e.g., age range 18-31 years) individuals. However, many of these studies failed to separate the role of visual guidance from motor performance. That is, subjects in the other studies were allowed to use vision to guide their movement responses. This is not a trivial problem; there is a sizable literature which documents perceptual-motor interaction (e.g., see Rosenbaum, 1991; Szafran, 1951). Accordingly, I will constrain the scope of this chapter to cover only those studies where the role of visual perception is limited to stimulus processing. Moreover, each of the studies reviewed assessed healthy, community dwelling elderly (age range 63-80 years) and young (age range 18-31 years) individuals. Also, subjects in these studies received sufficiently numerous trials to allow an assumption that both subjects attained their respective asymptotic levels of practice on the various tasks (see, e.g., Spirduso & MacRae, 1990, concerning differential practice effects preceding asymptotic performance).

Two popular aimed movement tasks in the aging literature are the movement plan specification and restructuring tasks. Common to both tasks is the manipulation of movement parameters such as arm, direction, extent, velocity, force, etc. Such parameters take on values which are specific to a generalized motor program that defines a particular pattern of physical activity (Schmidt, 1988). Latency to initiate (RT) and execute (MT) a planned movement is assessed, as well as errors which may occur for movement initiation and execution. Overall, both tasks have exhibited age-related slowing like that seen for the SRT/CRT tasks; this is not surprising, because these movement tasks also manipulate stimulus uncertainty in a fashion similar to SRT and CRT tasks (Amrhein et al., 1991). However, the age-related slowing observed in these movement tasks has rarely received statistical analysis beyond experiment-specific determination of elderly/young latency ratios (see Goggin & Stelmach, 1990). As will be detailed later, regression analysis across-experiments may offer a more detailed assessment of the loci of this slowing, specifically concerning “sensory-motor” and intervening “computational” processes (Cerella, 1990). Details of each task are presented below.

Movement plan specification task


The movement plan specification task involves a precuing paradigm in which the subject is given partial or complete parameter information concerning the impending movement response (e.g., use of the left arm in a movement away from oneself). RTs indicating additional latency to respond to a stimulus, relative to that for a completely specified response, are related to the parameters left to be specified for that stimulus after preceding partial specification of the other parameters by the precue stimulus (e.g., left arm is specified but direction – away or toward oneself – is...

PDFPDF (Adobe DRM)

Kopierschutz: Adobe-DRM
Adobe-DRM ist ein Kopierschutz, der das eBook vor Mißbrauch schützen soll. Dabei wird das eBook bereits beim Download auf Ihre persönliche Adobe-ID autorisiert. Lesen können Sie das eBook dann nur auf den Geräten, welche ebenfalls auf Ihre Adobe-ID registriert sind.
Details zum Adobe-DRM

Dateiformat: PDF (Portable Document Format)
Mit einem festen Seiten­layout eignet sich die PDF besonders für Fach­bücher mit Spalten, Tabellen und Abbild­ungen. Eine PDF kann auf fast allen Geräten ange­zeigt werden, ist aber für kleine Displays (Smart­phone, eReader) nur einge­schränkt geeignet.

Systemvoraussetzungen:
PC/Mac: Mit einem PC oder Mac können Sie dieses eBook lesen. Sie benötigen eine Adobe-ID und die Software Adobe Digital Editions (kostenlos). Von der Benutzung der OverDrive Media Console raten wir Ihnen ab. Erfahrungsgemäß treten hier gehäuft Probleme mit dem Adobe DRM auf.
eReader: Dieses eBook kann mit (fast) allen eBook-Readern gelesen werden. Mit dem amazon-Kindle ist es aber nicht kompatibel.
Smartphone/Tablet: Egal ob Apple oder Android, dieses eBook können Sie lesen. Sie benötigen eine Adobe-ID sowie eine kostenlose App.
Geräteliste und zusätzliche Hinweise

Buying eBooks from abroad
For tax law reasons we can sell eBooks just within Germany and Switzerland. Regrettably we cannot fulfill eBook-orders from other countries.

Mehr entdecken
aus dem Bereich
Psychotherapien wirksam gestalten

von Ulrich Schultz-Venrath

eBook Download (2013)
Klett-Cotta (Verlag)
38,99
Psychotherapien wirksam gestalten

von Ulrich Schultz-Venrath

eBook Download (2014)
Klett-Cotta (Verlag)
38,99